谷歌浏览器插件
订阅小程序
在清言上使用

Correction To: ‘how Do Developmental and Parental Exposures to Predation Affect Personality and Immediate Behavioural Plasticity in the Snail Physa Acuta ?'

Proceedings - Royal Society Biological sciences/Proceedings - Royal Society Biological Sciences(2021)

引用 10|浏览0
暂无评分
摘要
You have accessMoreSectionsView PDF ToolsAdd to favoritesDownload CitationsTrack Citations ShareShare onFacebookTwitterLinked InRedditEmail Cite this article Tariel J., Plénet S. and Luquet É. 2021Correction to: ‘How do developmental and parental exposures to predation affect personality and immediate behavioural plasticity in the snail Physa acuta?'Proc. R. Soc. B.2882021061920210619http://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2021.0619SectionYou have accessCorrectionsCorrection to: ‘How do developmental and parental exposures to predation affect personality and immediate behavioural plasticity in the snail Physa acuta?' J. Tariel J. Tariel http://orcid.org/0000-0003-1552-4088 Google Scholar Find this author on PubMed Search for more papers by this author , S. Plénet S. Plénet Google Scholar Find this author on PubMed Search for more papers by this author and É. Luquet É. Luquet http://orcid.org/0000-0002-9550-8899 Google Scholar Find this author on PubMed Search for more papers by this author J. Tariel J. Tariel http://orcid.org/0000-0003-1552-4088 Google Scholar Find this author on PubMed Search for more papers by this author , S. Plénet S. Plénet Google Scholar Find this author on PubMed Search for more papers by this author and É. Luquet É. Luquet http://orcid.org/0000-0002-9550-8899 Google Scholar Find this author on PubMed Search for more papers by this author Published:14 April 2021https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2021.0619See original article.How do developmental and parental exposures to predation affect personality and immediate behavioural plasticity in the snail Physa acuta?Proc. R. Soc. B287, 20201761 (Published Online 23 December 2020) (doi:10.1098/rspb.2020.1761)Most references are incorrect in the paragraph of the discussion ‘(a) Developmental and parental environments influenced group mean in personality and immediate plasticity'. This paragraph is given below with the corrected references in bold. The reference list is given afterwards.(a) Developmental and parental environments influenced group mean in personality and immediate plasticityContrary to our expectations, developmental and parental exposures to predator cues reduced group mean in immediate plasticity. More specifically, snails had on average a reduced anti-predator immediate plasticity if they or their parents were exposed to a risk of predation, to the extent that exposed snails from exposed parents (PP snails) were not plastic to immediate predator cues. Concerning group mean in personality, in the immediate predator-free environment, parental and developmental environments had no effect on group mean in personality (i.e. similar mean escape behaviour among offspring from developmental and parental environments). But in the immediate predator-cue environment, both developmental and parental exposures to predator cues induced on average a slower escape. Cues of the past presence of predators might be conflicting cues in an immediate predator-free environment, whereas they might give relevant information in a predator-cue environment. In the literature on transgenerational plasticity, there is conflicting experimental evidence regarding the direction of the effects of parental and developmental exposures to predator cues on anti-predator behaviour. Parental and developmental exposures increase mean anti-predator behaviour in most cases (e.g. [25,34,35]). This pattern is traditionally explained as preadaptation: if cues present at the parental generation or during development accurately predict the presence of predators in the future environment, individuals already exhibiting anti-predator behaviour are pre-adapted to the presence of predators. Conversely, parental and developmental exposures to predator cues can reduce mean anti-predator behaviour [16,20], as in our study. Snails may exhibit low anti-predator behaviour because they are already protected from predators by morphological defences (trait compensation (e.g. [33,58,59,60])), thus saving the costs of having both morphological and behavioural defences. Parental and developmental exposures to predator cues can induce the production of morphological defences in many species (e.g. [17,61]), including P. acuta (thicker shell [25]). However, some studies have rather shown that individuals with high morphological defences also have high anti-predator behaviour (trait co-specialization [62,63]). The compensation or co-specialization of behavioural and morphological defences may depend on the efficiency of the defences and predator density [64]. On the other hand, snails may exhibit low anti-predator behaviour because they are strongly habituated to predator cues (crayfish and alarm odours). Habituation is a simple form of learning that occurs when behaviour response to a persistent stimulus is reduced [65]. Habituation may persist a while after the stimulus has disappeared (long-term habituation [65]). In the context of predation, long-term habituation has been demonstrated after repeated exposure to harmless predator cues [66,67]. This habituation may involve sensory habituation, where olfactory receptors lose their sensitivity to the odour as the odour persist. This sensory habituation is thought to allow an animal to focus its cognitive resources on a new or changing odour and better respond to it [68]. This habituation may also involve an active and complex decision from a higher cognitive centre. This would allow the prey to stop regarding a cue as dangerous after a long period of time without being attacked, thus reducing the costs associated with anti-predator behaviour [69,70]. In our case, predator-exposed snails were subjected for 53 days to predator odour and behavioural assessments started a few days after exposure to the predator odour had ceased. Predator-exposed snails may have become habituated to this predator odour and may have reduced their response to a novel exposure of this same predator odour. However, transgenerational transmission of habituation has never been highlighted to our knowledge, even if the transgenerational transfer of conditioning or sensory imprinting to an odour have already been described in nematods, rodents and butterflies [19,36,37].Footnotes© 2021 The Author(s)Published by the Royal Society. All rights reserved.References16. Urszán TJ, Garamszegi LZ, Nagy G, Hettyey A, Török J, Herczeg G.2018Experience during development triggers between-individual variation in behavioural plasticity. J. Anim. Ecol. 87, 1264-1273. (doi:10.1111/1365-2656.12847) Crossref, PubMed, ISI, Google Scholar17. Agrawal AA, Laforsch C, Tollrian R.1999Transgenerational induction of defences in animals and plants. Nature 401, 60-63. (doi:10.1038/43425) Crossref, ISI, Google Scholar19. Dias BG, Ressler KJ.2014Parental olfactory experience influences behavior and neural structure in subsequent generations. Nat. Neurosci. 17, 89-96. (doi:10.1038/nn.3594) Crossref, PubMed, ISI, Google Scholar20. Donelan SC, Trussell GC.2018Parental and embryonic experiences with predation risk affect prey offspring behaviour and performance. Proc. R. Soc. B 285, 20180034. (doi:10.1098/rspb.2018.0034) Link, ISI, Google Scholar25. Luquet E, Tariel J.2016Offspring reaction norms shaped by parental environment: interaction between within- and trans-generational plasticity of inducible defenses. BMC Evol. Biol. 16, 209. (doi:10.1186/s12862-016-0795-9) Crossref, PubMed, ISI, Google Scholar33. DeWitt TJ, Sih A, Hucko JA.1999Trait compensation and cospecialization in a freshwater snail: size, shape and antipredator behaviour. Anim. Behav. 58, 397-407. (doi:10.1006/anbe.1999.1158) Crossref, PubMed, ISI, Google Scholar34. Storm JJ, Lima SL.2010Mothers forewarn offspring about predators: a transgenerational maternal effect on behavior. Am. Nat. 175, 382-390. (doi:10.1086/650443) Crossref, PubMed, ISI, Google Scholar35. Giesing ER, Suski CD, Warner RE, Bell AM.2011Female sticklebacks transfer information via eggs: effects of maternal experience with predators on offspring. Proc. R. Soc. B 278, 1753-1759. (doi:10.1098/rspb.2010.1819) Link, ISI, Google Scholar36. Remy J-J.2010Stable inheritance of an acquired behavior in Caenorhabditis elegans. Curr. Biol. 20, R877-R878. (doi:10.1016/j.cub.2010.08.013) Crossref, PubMed, ISI, Google Scholar37. Gowri V, Dion E, Viswanath A, Piel FM, Monteiro A.2019Transgenerational inheritance of learned preferences for novel host plant odors in Bicyclus anynana butterflies. Evolution 73, 2401-2414. (doi:10.1111/evo.13861) Crossref, PubMed, ISI, Google ScholarNew references58. Chivers DP, Zhao X, Ferrari MCO2007Linking morphological and behavioural defences: prey fish detect the morphology of conspecifics in the odour signature of their predators. Ethology 113, 733-739. (doi:10.1111/j.1439-0310.2006.01385.x) Crossref, ISI, Google Scholar59. Ahlgren J, Chapman BB, Nilsson PA, Bronmark C.2015Individual boldness is linked to protective shell shape in aquatic snails. Biol. Lett. 11, 20150029. (doi:10.1098/rsbl.2015.0029) Link, ISI, Google Scholar60. Dijk B, Laurila A, Orizaola G, Johansson F.2016Is one defence enough? Disentangling the relative importance of morphological and behavioural predator-induced defences. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 70, 237-246. (doi:10.1007/s00265-015-2040-8) Crossref, ISI, Google Scholar61. Bestion E, Teyssier A, Aubret F, Clobert J, Cote J.2014Maternal exposure to predator scents: offspring phenotypic adjustment and dispersal. Proc. R. Soc. B 281, 20140701. (doi:10.1098/rspb.2014.0701) Link, ISI, Google Scholar62. Mikolajewski DJ, Johansson F.2004Morphological and behavioral defenses in dragonfly larvae: trait compensation and cospecialization. Behav. Ecol. 15, 614-620. (doi:10.1093/beheco/arh061) Crossref, ISI, Google Scholar63. Marshall CA, Wund MA.2017The evolution of correlations between behavioural and morphological defence in Alaskan threespine stickleback fish (Gasterosteus aculeatus): evidence for trait compensation and co-specialization. Evol. Ecol. Res. 18, 305-322. ISI, Google Scholar64. Steiner UK, Pfeiffer T.2007Optimizing time and resource allocation trade-offs for investment into morphological and behavioral defense. Am. Nat. 169, 118-129. (doi:10.1086/509939) Crossref, PubMed, ISI, Google Scholar65. Christoffersen GR.1997Habituation: events in the history of its characterization and linkage to synaptic depression. A new proposed kinetic criterion for its identification. Prog. Neurobiol. 53, 45-66. (doi:10.1016/S0301-0082(97)00031-2) Crossref, PubMed, ISI, Google Scholar66. Deecke VB, Slater PJB, Ford JKB. 2002Selective habituation shapes acoustic predator recognition in harbour seals. Nature 420, 171-173. (doi:10.1038/nature01030) Crossref, PubMed, ISI, Google Scholar67. Hemmi JM, Merkle T.2009High stimulus specificity characterizes anti-predator habituation under natural conditions. Proc. R. Soc. B 276, 4381-4388. (doi:10.1098/rspb.2009.1452) Link, ISI, Google Scholar68. Das Set al.2011Plasticity of local GABAergic interneurons drives olfactory habituation. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 108, E646-654. (doi:10.1073/pnas.1106411108) Crossref, PubMed, ISI, Google Scholar69. Rodriguez-Prieto I, Fernández-Juricic E, Martín J, Regis Y.2009Antipredator behavior in blackbirds: habituation complements risk allocation. Behav. Ecol. 20, 371-377. (doi:10.1093/beheco/arn151) Crossref, ISI, Google Scholar70. Ferrari MCO, Elvidge CK, Jackson CD, Chivers DP, Brown GE.2010The responses of prey fish to temporal variation in predation risk: sensory habituation or risk assessment?Behav. Ecol. 21, 532-536. (doi:10.1093/beheco/arq023) Crossref, ISI, Google Scholar Previous ArticleNext Article FiguresRelatedReferencesDetailsRelated articlesHow do developmental and parental exposures to predation affect personality and immediate behavioural plasticity in the snail Physa acuta?Dec 23, 2020, 12:00:00 AMProceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences This Issue14 April 2021Volume 288Issue 1948 Article InformationDOI:https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2021.0619PubMed:33849319Published by:Royal SocietyPrint ISSN:0962-8452Online ISSN:1471-2954History: Published online14/04/2021Published in print14/04/2021 License:© 2021 The Author(s)Published by the Royal Society. All rights reserved. Citations and impact Subjectsbehaviourevolution Large datasets are available through Proceedings B's partnership with Dryad
更多
查看译文
关键词
Adaptive Evolution
AI 理解论文
溯源树
样例
生成溯源树,研究论文发展脉络
Chat Paper
正在生成论文摘要